Lostinidlewonder, I'll keep this brief...
The topic is thinking styles and their role in learning. Opening a thread does not by default appoint you as a chairperson, or permit you to pass ruling on the natural evolution of a discussion
If you feel that the balance between conscious thought and experimentation is an inappropriate tangent, then feel free not to reply to any points on this
(just as I'm not interested in addressing any of the details of your uber-tangent about tangents).
Discussion progresses- especially in such a broad topic as this one. If you are not interested by the self-evident relevance of the balance that necessarily occurs between conscious intent and freer experimentation, nobody is forcing you to either read my posts or respond to them.
I am going to carry on posting in this interesting thread.
If you are not interested in what I have to say
I'm not going to address your post
In your absense, myself and other posters have been addressing the subject of thinking styles. You say you don't like tangents, so please stop prolonging one.
Yes, but good sightreaders read unfamiliar chords and combinations well. It's not memory of chunks so much as rapid usage of basic principles. .
I disagree. Well, I agree that this is a regurgitation of conventional wisdom. It is probably shared by a large number of teachers and players.But after considerable thought and discussion with my peers over the past couple of years, I've concluded it is wrong, and changed my practice habits accordingly. (It isn't completely wrong. The processing component exists along with the retrieval component. But I believe that a) the retrieval component is the majority and b) the retrieval component is what the good sightreaders do differently) Knowing that I can't convince you, and in fact am unlikely even to get you to consider the possibility, I will now end my attempts to explain this further.
To reiterate, the example of dab vs bad etc proves beyond any conceivable doubt that bigger units can only be processed due to recognition of fine detail.
If this is my dab vs. bad, then it involves something else. This involves a disorder of not being able to recognize things spacially in a visual manner. I got trained in this as a teacher, and I also have a mild version of this myself. It's like being colour-blind. You can study green and blue however long you want, if you don't see it, you don't see it! The solution is not study of detail, but using different senses.
Niyereghazi, I would not have thought that recognizing learning styles or strengths and weaknesses implies pandering to them. That would be simplistic. Not to say that that is what might be done. I would think that it is more a matter of being aware that there is such a thing, and then working with what is there. When I taught in the classroom, I made sure that my presentations were both visual and audial - i.e. you use pictures and you explain. Since the kids were young and young people are physical, we included movement. One child who had a particular problem was given an old fashioned typewriter (suggested by a specialist who came in) and when he was forced to slow down and type out his spelling words, for some reason the combination of those actions jogged his brain into being able to spell. You do what works.
Sure- I think my point is a lot to do with short-term vs long-term. Individual strengths are probably best for short-term learning. But if you do not challenge someone to develop other areas, that "strength" soon turns into a source of long-term weakness in the other areas. It only serves to fuel dependence on that area and neglect of others- unless there is a broader and more general approach.
I If you can recognise dab and bad when written in isolation (not in the context of other words), there are only two senses at play that could enable this to be happening. These are sight and the "sense" of left vs. right.
. But if you do not challenge someone to develop other areas, that "strength" soon turns into a source of long-term weakness in the other areas. It only serves to fuel dependence on that area and neglect of others- unless there is a broader and more general approach.
The problem is that those of us with this difficulty CANNOT do this consistently, because that sense is not there and it cannot be developed. First you have to look at the actual needs in playing piano. Then you can identify a real goal. The need is to be able to locate and play correct notes, move along correct intervals, and therefore have some way of recognizing these things. The need is not to specifically be able to look at an array of keys with your eyes, and find these things. Therefore, if other senses will get you there, you use them. If by closing my eyes and feeling the keys until their topography become familiar by touch, I can get there, then that is what I should use. It would be stupid to use a defective sense where something isn't there.
How do you explain ANY ability to distinguish between bs and ds, if the sense does not exist in you?
I stated that the part that is fault involves looking with the eyes. Other sense can be used. In my case touch, hearing (for piano - higher and lower sounds) work. For some reason mentally visualizing also works. This is my TRAINING as a teacher in a specialist area. Using different senses or addressing different senses is important. That is why in some learning disabilities, kids learn their alphabet by tracing letters that have been cut out on sandpaper. Once they have absorbed the shape using a sense that works properly for them, they can then transfer that concept of the letter's shape and use it.
You're missing my point. The letters b and d are shaped identically. Their orientation with regard to the left and right is the only distinguishing feature. Faiing to develop awareness (be it conscious or unconscious)
I am writing from the perspective of specialized training in learning disabilities, having worked with students with such disabilities, and solutions toward the same. I have stated three times over that the way to get at "developing this awareness" comes through using senses other than looking at it, and recognizing that distinction by looking at it. Are you not following that some people may need to use the sense of touch rather than sight in order to get at this awareness?
Niyereghazi, the perception of left and right, or whatever lets you distinguish left from right, cannot process through looking. Something doesn't function as it does in most people. If you have a sense, it is very hard to imagine what it is like not to have it. For a long time education was in the dark ages and called these people "lazy" because they were obviously intelligent. The SOLUTION is to use other senses. I have this in a mild form. You cannot get this through thinking or studying. It simply is not there. In that sense it is like colour blindness. THE IMPORTANT THING IS TO KNOW WHAT SOLUTIONS EXIST if you are a teacher. Substituting touch is one way of getting at direction.
If you can recognise dab and bad when written in isolation (not in the context of other words), there are only two senses at play that could enable this to be happening. These are sight and the "sense" of left vs. right.
I have tried several times to share the teaching approaches that I acquired through specialized training, then practice and experience, and how it helps with a particular difficulty. If you find any of it useful, then feel free to use or adapt it. There is absolutely nothing defeatist about saying there are other ways of going about acquiring certain things.
Ok, succinctly - How have you gone about helping students with these kinds of learning disabilities solve these kinds of problems? I have told you both my training, and my experiences. There has been a lot of success. Can you share what you have done in the same area, and what has worked? This is a teacher forum so we are interested in practical applications, not just theoretical constructs. If you have a student with this difficulty, how will you help him?
Part of what you have invented just now is how it is taught. The problem is that left and right or the shapes cannot be recognized visually so you cannot get at any trick for recognizing it later. I.e. "looking" at it continues to "look the same". But if you can get a first handle on it, you can get somewhere. Therefore different ways could be feeling a plasticine d and b, creating a plasticine d and b, having someone trace the letters on your back, tracing it in the air. that is similar to your proprioception. Once you have that first sense, then you can deal with that d-ness and b-ness. That is how it is done.
The observation comes from feeling the shape. Left and right are not only known through vision. They are known through other senses.
Indeed. That's the point I raised. That you have to know them (by ANY means) and then use that to understand the single issue that makes d different from b- i.e. how left and right work. If a student can't take this beyond feeling and transfer it into the visual, it will never be possible to distinguish between b and d with any ease.
I guess my question was why reference to piano often seems almost exclusively visual. Intervals may be distances in keys, but they are also sounds with particular qualities that we can hear. Higher and lower notes are not only to the right and left, but also pitch differences that we hear. It is not only visual.I was greatly puzzled by what a teacher said a few years ago in another forum. She described a beginner's playing of the piano as being a case of seeing notes on a page, pressing down the keys, and then finding out what sound would be heard. After playing the same piece for a week or two, the student would begin to anticipate what some of it would sound like. For me, after a very short time as a child, I knew what I would hear before playing written notes. When I pressed down the keys, I was aiming for that sequence of notes. It was not much different from singing. How can you not hear at least a melody in your head when you look at notes? In the description I read, sound played no role whatsoever.
I came in through a back door. The only education I received as a child was a bit of solfege in some primary grade, probably grade 2. The teacher had a board with the solfege syllables written vertically, would point at notes, and we would sing. A lot of them were patterns that we often find in classical music. So I had a kind of aural template in my head and I sang. When I played other instruments, I aimed for the same thing. Then when I had a piano and was given notation, I found "Do" sang along it, and produced the notes. My first pieces were Clementi sonatinas. There was some thin 10-page booklet before that which showed a few chords which I played and heard in my head after that. So for me, sound was linked to notation and the piano from the very beginning.This is one reason why I have had to get a map of the keyboard into my head. For me the sounds simply emanated from inside the piano, as if shining forth from their locations. I was barely aware of black or white keys - just where the sound was. This was both an advantage and a handicap.I also think that players of different instruments will relate to these things in different ways. Brass players have the harmonic series and have to picture a sound before they can play it because there is no physical location. String players have another relationship again. There are physical locations, but you can find the same pitch on a different string at a different location. For an unfretted string instrument, you must have some kind of ear from day one.
I don't play a brass instrument. Do you?
I know you can get somewhere by a combination of feel and sound on piano because I have done it. You feel where the sound is and you go there. Only you also tend to hover closer to the keys, which you also see blind pianists do. There is less freedom of motion.
The explanations already fall apart with phonetic words being written as they are pronounced. There are too many shifts in pronunciation and I don't mean dialects or regionalism. There is no such thing as "white English". Even that varies in North America, and pronunciation continues to shift.
But the biggest thing for me is the idea that there is an either or. We use all approaches, and part of one is also embedded in the other.
There is such a thing as “white English”, as long as you don’t ask a white person what that means. White people generally don’t know they speak white (as they usually don’t experience what is known as the contrast effect.)
allows the perpetuation of naive assumptions and makes it easier, for the teacher, to teach the way they were taught and assert that “it worked for me and look how I turned out” or to adopt scientific evidence to pre-existing ideas.
You seem to be writing about studies happening in the United States, and educational practices happening in that country. I can only speak about my own country which is north of your border.
... That is not how my teacher training went, and not how my peers thought when I started out. Nor did I stay there. It does not reflect what I know of language learning in the field or in theory.
It seems that you want to defend your past practices based on your experiences which were based on a paradigm. ........... many past educational practices should be done away with. But, like old dogs, teachers can't seem to learn any new tricks.
... In the beginning with students my energy is spent on how this student thinks, responds, and communicates. Then I teach them music.
I have not read any studies specifically about memorization of basic arithmetic and overall general mathematical competency but they are probably out there, I just haven't read them yet. (Or maybe I have and don't remember.) However, using all the available research I have read and understand, your thinking is correct. The faster students are at calculating basic arithmetic (+,-, x, and /) the better they should be at math in general. The key is faster, aka: SPEED. This requires memorization and regurgitation of math facts. However, teachers rarely have students practice hundreds of arithmetic problems when they learned how to +,-,x, and /. Yet the learning research suggests it is absolutely vital.
The faster students are at calculating basic arithmetic (+,-, x, and /) the better they should be at math in general. The key is faster, aka: SPEED. This requires memorization and regurgitation of math facts. However, teachers rarely have students practice hundreds of arithmetic problems when they learned how to +,-,x, and /.