Look for film footage of nyiregyhazi on youtube and listen to mosonyis funeral March and the Liszt rhapsody no 3. I don't believe that's only down to illusions and nothing else. He made almost every sound from contact. He plays louder than most people could play by punching the keys. And he makes a huge sound before he even starts trying. Sometimes his very loudest gets percussive, but the sounds that fascinate me are the effortlessly massive ones that he makes without even coming close to his limits. Those who try to recreate such depth almost always start trying trying to hit or to move as faster. But they get a percussive tone and simply max out, before even approaching the level of volume or resonance that he reaches in his moderate dynamics, with no effort at all. I don't see any explanation at all other than the pacing of acceleration.
To speak of him moving keys "faster" than other pianists couldn't explain the first thing and neither could anyone recreate his sound with such a mindset. Pianists who think it's about moving faster hit their upper limit without getting anywhere near how far a piano can go (while still avoiding the hard percussive thuds). Volodos is another excellent example of a pianist who can play far louder than the point where most people assume it will sound percussive.
May I throw in another factor? I haven't seen anything here about the POINT OF SOUND. (Is that the so-called "let-off" in English?)In keyboards, for example, the point of sound seems to be set at the bottom of the key, but it doesn't work that way in a real piano, where that point is slightly higher. What about the timing of the keystroke into the heart of that point of sound, slightly lower, slightly higher, etc.? Isn't that where all the variations in sound QUALITY come from? Isn't that why a "relaxed" touch gives a much more beautiful tone, while a tense movement aimed at the bottom of the key (and thus "missing the point", haha) will cause a harsh tone? When you are "relaxed" and you "drop" your finger into the point of sound, you simply land where you have to be without interfering (like a cat that always lands on its four) and the instrument will play itself so to speak, something that will never happen by aiming with tension. Just saying.
this makes sense. if your intended point of sound (i.e. where you think the point of sound is as you are playing a key) doesn't line up with the actual point of sound, the velocity of your finger at contact will not be what you intended it to be. of course the sound that comes out will not be what you intially desired.
If this is indeed true, then the problem becomes the following: many pianists simply don't know what to intend or what to desire. They miss the sensitivity to regulate and adjust their touch, because they have no sound purpose other than loud, soft and in between. The reactions of the instrument itself leave them cold. That's why a beautiful touch is so rarely heard these days. Smiley
In fact Dima's original point is wrong. There's no evidence whatsoever, AFAIK, that playing a single isolated note with a stiff hand creates a harsher sound than playing it with a relaxed hand at the same velocity. What the stiff hand does do is limit the player's ability to precisely control that velocity.
Uhm. I have no problem with being proven wrong, but with what you say, you are basically denying that there is such a thing like the ratio between a fundamental or core tone (whatever you call that in English) and a certain overtone signature, caused by the resulting vibration of resonators in the instrument and even of objects in the room in a certain unique and optimal way at X decibels.
I have trouble accepting that this is a matter of hammer velocity only because my ears tell me otherwise. I suspect that good testing with accurate and adequate equipment for the purpose would reveal that there is more to this.
The only thing I'm saying is that in the case of the piano, changing the velocity of the keystroke is the only way we have of obtaining that difference.
All you would need to do is create a mechanism that can strike the key with different degrees of suddenness, while achieving a set, measurable velocity at the end of the hammer's travel, record the effects of two different strokes that achieve the same velocity and examine the wave form.
I don't see how one can judge loudness via a youtube clip played through a computer. But no matter; I agree with you that one pianist can create a greater psychological impression of loudness than another, playing the same passage of music on the same piano in the same room. However, I think it's perfectly possible to explain this via control of velocity alone (plus of course legato, pedalling etc. that I think we all agree on as factors but see as separate from the argument about the keystroke itself).It comes down to control of dynamic range.Anyone who has put a highly produced pop music CD in their player and played it, after playing a classical CD and not changing the volume, will understand this. The pop CD sounds many times louder even though, if you look at the very loudest sounds, it can't be. Both CDs are normalised to 0db and there's simply no way they can be louder than that. The difference is that the classical CD has an enormous dynamic range from PPP to fff, whereas the pop CD has been compressed to all hell to bring the softer sounds up to near the level of the louder ones. Anyone who's worked in music technology will tell you that one of the functions of such compression (when done in certain ways) is not just to make the music sound louder, but to make it sound "warmer", "richer", or "smoother" - terms that correspond pretty well to what most people here probably mean by "good tone".Suppose you get someone to play a big, thundering piece of romantic music on a Steinway grand in a particular concert hall. Now suppose that we could analyse the velocity of every keystroke, and map them on a scale from 1 - 100 (rounding them off to the nearest integer). Suppose we find that some impression of harshness starts to happen at around 85, and becomes very noticeably and unpleasant above about 90.Now, your highly skilled famous concert pianist playing the piece, will aim to get most of the loudest notes at about 84, maybe occasionally exceeding that a little and allowing a tiny bit of harshness judiciously placed according to the phrasing. Just as importantly, they will aim to get most of the other notes not too far below that, using just enough variation to obtain the sense of phrasing and dynamic shading that they need, while maintaining a consistently loud sound (plus of course any sections or voices that are deliberately intended to sound very soft, and don't contribute to the sense of loudness).If OTOH you get a second rate amateur to play the same piece, they will probably AIM to do the same thing. But instead of getting the louder notes right at 84, many of them will come out at 90 or higher and sound harsh, due to the player's lack of control. And instead of getting the softer notes at 75-80, many of them will come out at 60, 50 or lower for the same reason. The effect will be much "looser" and less consistent.Returning to the pop music compression example, one thing we know about psychoacoustics is that tightly controlled sounds within a narrow dynamic range give a greater subjective impression of "loudness" (AND of "warmth", "richness" etc.) than less controlled sounds over a wider range. In this case as well, the average dynamic of the amateur will surely be lower for the simply reason that if you're aiming for 75-84, and 100 corresponds to how hard it's realistically possible to thump a key in an actual playing situation, there is more scope for accidentally obtaining a lower dynamic due to poor technique, than a higher one. When the amateur hears harshness, he'll back off, but unlike the professional he won't have the control to back off by exactly the right amount and still sound loud.There are of course other factors involved too, like the professional's control of the pedal etc. And of course as soon as you are talking about chords (ie, nearly all the time) then control of different simultaneous dynamics comes into play as well as sequential ones. Harshness is something we mostly experience in higher frequencies, and the extent to which a higher note sounds harsh will be mitigated by being supported by simultaneous lower notes. Someone with the precision of technique to get those lower notes to exactly the right level of support will be better placed to raise the level of the higher notes a bit further.You suffer here from the same problem that your claim always seems to suffer from. You have made a claim about the nature of individual keystrokes, but you are illustrating it with an example of the playing of an actual piece, with many keystrokes interacting. Once you consider such interaction, it's perfectly possible to explain the phenomenon you're talking about without recourse to unfounded voodoo about hammer flexion, which may or may not even be happening to a significant degree in the first place.These things - the psychological effect of dynamic compression; the more precise technique of the better player - these are known phenomena for which there is clear evidence. They are perfectly sufficient (along with the other factors I mentioned) to explain the difference in subjective impression of loudness when hearing two different pianist play the same piece, and Occam's razor tells us not to multiply explanatory phenomena beyond necessity.To actually prove your point, you'd need to get nyiregyhazi to play a SINGLE NOTE at a loud dynamic but without "harshness", and then fashion some kind of mechanism that could reproduce exactly the same hammer velocity but with a quick, sudden stroke of the key from above. If there were a discernible difference to the listener in double blind tests, in nyiregyhazi's favour, then you'd have a case.Clearly it's not all about moving faster overall, but about fine control of fast movements and how they interact in the piece. Again, when you make this judgment of Volodos you are judging the overall effect of a piece, not the specific effect of a single note.
The result should also be evaluated by qualified people in the hall, and not by just anyone through a recording or wave graphics. Every good pianist knows that his/her tone, even the softest whisper, should "carry" to the farthest corner and this is not exclusively a matter of soft or loud. This quality of a good tone can hardly be captured through a recording. You can't get reliable test results with machines on either side because they will "miss the point".
These postulations would be all very well, but clearly you didn't actually listen to nyiregyhazi first.
The idea that nyiregyhazi is simply playing quieter than amateurs who supposedly sound harsh by reaching a higher velocity than him is plain comical in its absurdity. You don't get that huge intensity in your mid-range by playing quieter than amateurs.
As I said, I heard a concert where a college student played so roughly that I could actually distinguish the thump of key on keybed. She didn't come close to what nyiregyhazi produces without a trace of thud. Horowitz exploited plenty of relativity, but nyiregyhazi genuinely makes a much bigger resonance in his moderate dynamics and also finds room for more still. That girl's sound wasn't big or loud at all. At a professional level, Kissin's sound has that harsh thump at levels that are spectacular short of nyiregyhazi's volume. Your argument is based primarily on the fallacy that the existence of relative issues might in any way disprove absolute ones. They are not mutually exclusive, so this does not follow.
Intensity of tone and loudness of sound are not the same thing.
What do you mean by "intensity of tone"?
It's not a problem with pressing. That's a worse problem still. But doing nothing doesn't bring the hand into the next position. It squashes the hand into a position that requires all the more muscular effort than opening up properly and standing on the thumb. If you fall down and sag into your thumb, your fingers are left behind in the last position. When the thumb expands the arch open, they are lifted over the top and automatically brought into into the next position. No amount of passivity does that for you. You can do exercises to wipe the slate clean first and remove general tension before focusing on the necessary activitied. But if the thumb doesn't act to automatically lift your fingers over the top and into their next position, it takes vastly more muscular effort to being them around in a deliberate way. You have to learn how to open up the arch in the hand before striving to stay close can be effective. Droop in the arch will always cause stiffness to compensate for the instability that comes with it. It's like trying to "relax" by squatting over an imaginary chair instead of standing upright. Elevating your knuckles is less effort than letting them droop shut.
PS I found a solution for students whose fingers rise uncontrollably. It's a "panic" response to instability, like throwing your arms out if about to fall over. Exaggerate the movements even further at first. Deliberately lift every finger out and then carry on standing properly on the last finger with high knuckles when you let them back down. You'll become more stable and the fingers will stop doing the panic movements by themselves.
No, you're wrong here.
OK I've listened to it now. It hasn't changed my opinion of anything I wrote above.Sorry but I don't get how you can make claims about absolute volume based on a recording. If I turn my speakers down, then he is playing quietly. If I turn them up, then he's playing loudly. If I listen to him and then listen to a different pianist who was recorded with a more sensitive microphone placed closer to the piano and the recording has been normalised higher, then they sound louder. You make no sense here. The only way to compare the loudness of two pianists is by making all factors external to their touch - the piano, the room etc - the same.Did you by chance actually READ my post before responding to it? A subjective impression like "huge intensity" is not the same thing as the technical comparison of the volume of specific sounds. What exactly do you mean - or think you mean - when you describe nyiregyhazi's playing as "louder" than other people's? Do you mean that the loudest notes he plays are louder than the loudest notes they do? Do you mean that the AVERAGE loudness of what he plays is louder than the AVERAGE of what they do? And how could this even be meaningful when he might play some passages louder than them, and they play others louder than him? Do you mean that every single note he plays is louder than every note they do?I think I know the answer - which is simply that you come away with a greater SUBJECTIVE IMPRESSION of GENERAL loudness. Which is fine, except that you seem completely unwilling to consider what the relationship is between that subjective impression and the actual objectively measurable qualities of the sounds coming from the piano. But when we're arguing about whether a single note can be played with a measurably different timbre when struck with the same velocity, you don't get to do that.It's completely possible that an amateur could play some of the notes in the piece, even many of them, louder than nyiregyhazi, while still giving an overall impression of softer playing. Subjective impression of loudness is NOT created simply by the loudest transients; it's created by the general level of volume combined with the degree to which ALL the sounds are pushed up towards that volume. This is a well known psycho-acoustical fact. It's going to be especially the case here since the difference in loudness between a note just below the harshness threshold and one above it might be quite small, and most importantly largely due to a few milliseconds of the initial attack alone. It might not even register to the listener consciously as a difference in loudness.The relationship between loudness and frequency would come into it too, since as I said before harshness is not experienced as a function of velocity equally across the frequency spectrum. So if he has the technical command to reduce velocity just enough to avoid harshness to different degrees appropriate to the particular range of the piano involved, then he can perfectly well give a lesser impression of harshness while playing louder overall.And your argument is based on the fallacy that your SUBJECTIVE IMPRESSION of "loudness" corresponds lock, stock and barrel to some objective phenomenon whereby he is "playing louder", without even needing to specify what that phenomenon is. Again, you need to educate yourself in the psycho-acoustical facts of how this works, because what you are writing here is simply ignorant.To phrase it in terms of my (admittedly crude) velocity measurements: if he is playing everything between a velocity of 75-84, and an amateur is playing between a velocity of 40 - 95, with 85-90 being the point where harshness is experienced, then you will hear his performance as "louder" even though his loudest attacks are softer than theirs. The control which he applies to every note in the piece to extract maximum loudness from it, subject to the demands of phrasing, will have much more effect upon your psychological judgment of loudness than the relative levels of a few outstanding transients. Even moreso once you consider the role of voicing of chords. He may even play each chord louder in terms of its impression as a whole unit (which is how we hear it), while playing the specific frequencies that contribute most to harshness softer.Your other point about key thump I will have to come back to as it's a separate issue and I don't have time right now.
I still don't get it. Why not start with proper hand position, certain degree of arch, and maintain it, always observing visually as well as the sound of the notes for evenness etc? I find no need to make my thumb press down more to make others raise going over, otherwise a collapsed hand. I may not be understanding you so don't get me wrong, just trying to get your point.I don't find the fingers rising uncontrollably because of a panic. I think it is weakness of certain fingers in certain patterns, say 3-4-5 where when 4 pressed, 5 pops up. It is a symptom of other muscles trying to compensate for a finger not being able at a given speed to act independently of the others. Proof is slowing it way down, viola! finger remains on the keys like they should. More time needed with only the smallest muscles involved with finger depression independent of others. Then the control increases. Visual + sound = success.Nick
Nobody will ever play a truly fast scale when they drop into the thumb. It's one of the ultimate hallmark of awkard amateur pianism and something that no top pro will ever be seen doing. The arm just drifts along. You cannot fall down into the thumb in a good scale and expect to have speed and control.
Ah, but at slow tempos instability can be corrected with arm tensions. Only when you go fast does the integrity of every individual finger need to be near perfect. Everything is easy if you go slow enough. The question is whether slow work trains the fingers to stabilise better, or whether you keep depending on tensions.
Regarding the thumb, as I said before, after passing the thumb you're stuck in the last position if the thumb doesn't lift the fingers over it. That makes getting into the next position more complex and more physical effort to find. When the thumb lifts them over they just get where they need to be. They realign as if by magic around the thumb. Plus the whole arm has to drop in the thumb if it doesn't actually generate movement. Nobody will ever play a truly fast scale when they drop into the thumb. It's one of the ultimate hallmark of awkard amateur pianism and something that no top pro will ever be seen doing. The arm just drifts along. You cannot fall down into the thumb in a good scale and expect to have speed and control.
I don't have arm tension at slower tempos, am not depending on arm tensions. There is a certain fixation of the arm in order to prevent the hand from rising when the finger depresses a key, but this is only the amount needed to prevent that, takes very little. Proof is the speed over time of scale like passages is increasing with no change in feel, tension etc. I am not stuck in the last position even though my thumb is not lifting my fingers over it. It is arm movement that allows this. Picture this: hand poised over the keys without depressing, and moving up and down the keyboard as though one were a concert pianist actually playing the notes in a scale. This is the movement I strive for and one does not "fall down into the thumb". Likewise, when the fingers each are trained to play independently, each not affecting the others, it feels similarly to the above example. I just don't play as you describe if I understand you correctly and am ok with that. To each his own. We possibly could go on and on until the cows come home and there would be no change. Good luck and thanks for sharing your thoughts. Nick
Is the movement absolutely steady and continuous or does it come in sudden movements?
I am not convinced yet.You may be interested to know that this kind of discussions also used to be held about the voice of certain singers and their ability to be heard over an orchestra in the back of the hall. People who recognized such beautiful voices by ear were also deemed to be "fooling themselves" until the so-called singer-formant was found in the sound spectrum of great singers, a clear formant around 3000 Hz (between 2800 and 3400 Hz) that is absent in speech and in the sound spectra of UNTRAINED and bad singers, however hard they shout. The formant is formed as the result of resonance in certain parts of the vocal tract and has nothing to do with volume of tone.When I hear a pianist with a beautiful tone who easily penetrates a rather noisy orchestra but apparently does nothing, I get that same kind of aha-reflex: must be a formant-kind of thing because most pianists simply can't do it. Although the human ear is limited in its range of what it can catch, within that range, it outdoes any existing and advanced equipment. If we want reliable test results then, we should first analyze the sound spectrum of piano tone that is considered beautiful and determine what exactly makes it "beautiful" as compared to the sound spectrum of tone that is considered "poor". Do you know of any such research?
nyiregyhazi -I'm getting the feeling that you either haven't read my posts above or don't understand them, because I've explained quite clearly, using only known acoustical facts, why velocity alone is capable to achieving the effects you describe. You also completely ignored my questions about what you actually mean by "louder" and what you think the relationship between this subjective impression and the objective measurement of amplitude of each individual keystroke is. Until you do that, all you're referring to is a subjective impression and the known facts of acoustics DO support the claim that that impression can be created while the loudest transients are softer. Your incredulity about that seems to just be due to ignorance of the subject, so I can't really add any more.Even the engineer's comments about the upper levels of his playing don't contradict this, at all. How recording engineers manage a generally high sound level is a completely different question from how they manage a few very loud transients. And anyway, the salient factor in both cases would be the loudness of the chord, not the single keystroke. So voicing would matter, hugely.You seem to still be insisting on a highly reductionist paradigm whereby your subjective impression of loudness must be analyzable into a multitude of tiny units that are ALL themselves louder. Once again, this is simply ignorant of the science involved - it's like people who used to think that the Earth must be flat because every little bit of it that they can see is flat. If you can't take that science on board without utter disbelief that such a paradigm might be faulty, then I can't really go any further.I only didn't have time to address your question of key thump because it's a change of subject, and required thinking about in its own right. You've spent most of this exchange talking about the effect of hammer and shaft flexion on the hammer, potentially transferring itself to the strings, which is a different issue.Off the top of my head - yes, it sounds highly plausible that keythump contributes to the subjective impression of harshness. I don't know whether a recorded output would be able to distinguish between that effect and the note itself. Since the thump is noise and the note is pitched sound, it may well do. But I'd need to think about it or read about it some more.
Absolutely steady and continuous.Nick
A large part of this discussion, to the limited extent I can be bothered to read it, seems to be supremely irrelevant. However, there is of course a very simple proof that factors apart from the speed of the hammer can have an effect: if the pianist hasn't cut his/her fingernails in a while, you can hear the results at the back of the hall. QED. I still maintain that by far the most important consideration is simply relative loudness of notes.
"Returning to the excitation of the string by the hammer impact, not only the amplitude of the initial pulse on the string changes with the strength of the blow, but also its shape. This is due to a remarkable property of the felt hammer, more specifically the characteristics of its stiffness. The stiffness increases (the hammer becomes progressively harder to compress) the more the hammer already has been compressed, a phenomenon referred to as nonlinear stiffness. This means that a harder blow not only will give a larger amplitude but also sharper corners of the pulse on the string. Again, according to Fourier, sharper wiggles in the waveform correspond to more prominent high frequency partials in the spectrum. Consequently, the piano tone will attain a different ("more brilliant") tone quality at forte (loud) compared to piano (soft)."thoughts?and by the way, nygeraizi the thumping part i think has something to do with the player not aiming accurately at the point of sound. if the player aimed past the point of sound, thinking it is at the very bottom of the key, he might maximize the velocity of his finger stroke at the bottom of the key. This would "miss" the right point of sound, hitting it with a lower intended velocity than planned and producing a weaker sound while hitting the dead bottom of the key hard, producing an audible thump
where the finger is collapsing rather than expanding.
Collapse is obviously bad, but why oh why do you have this fetish for expansion? Contraction is better than expansion.
I'll be convinced when you post something that is both virtuoso and artistic.
Only in the knuckle. Contracting anything else in the finger sends it into a horrible collision with a dead stop. And it doesn't stop that hole in the transmission. If the fingertip is scratching backwards, the knuckle will still be descending faster than the fingertip if you use arm power. You waste movement on something that achieves no key acceleration but which instead puts energy into the aftershock that occurs after the hammer has been thrown loose, but when the knuckle is still spiralling downwards with energy that can only contribute to an impact. My fingertips also used to hurt in the joint when I gripped during big chords. Only the knuckle can afford to be closing for the most efficient transmission of power. The rest of the finger needs to straighten that out. The other benefit of expanding between tip and knuckle is that hard collision is effectively impossible. Any extra movement is AWAY from the key bed, not into it.
Just try it for yourself.
Ny -Do you have any links to source material about your Nyiregyhazi story?I don't really understand it - it doesn't make sense the way you've related it. Engineers record very loud sound sources all the time, louder ones than acoustic pianos. If they're not getting a clear signal without distortion they move the mic further away, use a different mic or turn the gain down. The idea that the difference in sound level between one pianist and all the other pianists who have played that piano in that room is so great that it blows all their recording procedures out of the water is just ridiculous. And it doesn't form a convincing basis for your presenting your own anti-scientific interpretation of your subjective impression as some kind of fact; or of your insistence on rejecting the actual science involved.But point me tot something and I'll read it. It sounds interesting.
Given how much you had been open-minded about earlier, I cannot understand why you'd now take two steps backwards and come up with such an implausible explanation for the whole thing- rather than consider the perfectly plausible possibility of these issues. I'm well aware of relativity. I didn't bother responding in detail, because it's quite such a poor explanation for a truly remarkable sound and because existence of relativity does not disprove additional factors. Relativity explains many things but doesn't scratch the surface here. He doesn't use the relativity idea of making moderate dynamics to softer to leave room for an illusion of louder forte. He starts extremely loud, at similar levels where ordinary pianists already pound with all their might and sound harsh. Then he gets louder.
Ah...I was right. You completely misunderstood my point, in fact you've actually reversed it.I didn't say that he used relativity by "making moderate dynamics softer". I said the OPPOSITE of that - exactly, indeed, what you're saying here.A compressor does not expand the dynamic range; it compresses it. (The clue is in the name ). In other words, it brings the soft and moderate sounds up to a level closer to the loud ones. The well known effect of this is an increased psychological perception of loudness overall, even when the loudest sounds are not made any louder. In fact depending on all the variables involved, that perception can ensue even when the loudest sounds are made softer.What I've been trying to describe is the potentially similar effect of a great concert pianist's control over the dynamics of every single keystroke. I won't repeat it here because it's all there in my posts above, particularly the one at the beginning of all this, after you posted the link. You could go back and read what I actually wrote rather than what you think I wrote (which it actually the opposite of what I wrote) if you're interested.The reality is that your subjective impression of loudness is a psychological phenomenon, determined much more by the loudness of the softest sounds than by the loudness of the loudest sounds. What you've written here about the way he played makes perfect sense in that respect. As does your complete misunderstanding that has so drawn out this exchange.
You can't just use the old nothing is objective on any level get-out here.
Nyiregyhazi was a very loud pianist and that's not subjective. It's not a scam. He played at louder levels than average pianists could reach without thumping out a percussive tone. He could push the piano further than other players and without even trying, before percussion became an issue.
This is not more ridiculous than the truly comedic theory that merely because things are subjective, the man that many regard as the loudest pianist ever captured on record secretly plays much quieter than any feeble amateur who is regarded as playing with a percussive edge. That ludicrous suggestion is simply not on the ball.
Schoenberg spoke of having never heard such a level of sonority Years before he was recorded. Illusions aren't enough to casually explain this away, sorry.
The CD Nyiregyhazi at the Opera is not compressed. Your assertions are pointless if you're not out there and listening and trying to recreate the sound minus the amateur thuds that only a rare expert will achieve the the absence of. They compressed it because it was the only way to press the LPs, not because any illusion needed creating. You're simply looking for excuses to try to write off what you are not interested in opening your mind about. There's an objective difference here.
When did I say that? A statement that something "sounds loud" is a statement of psychological impression. That's simply a fact. Whatever objective processes are involved in leading the person to that impression are what they are. I've actually been trying to clarify what those may be. You OTOH seem peculiarly resistent to doing that.Anyway, you STILL haven't clarified what you actually MEAN by "he was a loud pianist", as I've asked you to do twice in order to move the discussion forward. So I don't really buy the preaching of objectivity. What you're calling objectivity is simply your refusal to entertain anything but the most childish assumptions about what the objective phenomena must be that are causing your subjective impression.You do know I haven't actually denied this, right? That is, if we start with a clear idea of what "loud" actually means. And you DO know that I never actually said this, right?You DO know that when a pianist plays a chord, he doesn't necessarily play every note exactly the same way, right?You DO know that it's the total impression of the chord that we hear and react to with an impression of loudness, not its constituent parts, right?Good thing I never said anything about illusions then, but only about psychology and acoustical science. I will look up the youtube documentary though, thanks.
OH JESUS CHRIST.I didn't say Nyiregyhazi's sound was due to being compressed. I used the example of a compressor to illustrate how reducing the dynamic range of a sound spectrum can give a greater impression of loudness. Then I explained how a great pianist's technique could do the same thing. You would know this if you actually read my posts rather than just arguing with the sound of your own voice. For Christ's sake, you even AGREED with me here that that is precisely what he's doing: bringing up the level of the softer sounds.
you seem kinda biased toward the spectacularness of nyiregyhazi's sound.. after all, your forum name is nyiregyhazi
It's important to remember that we don't judge notes as harsh because they ARE harsh - because of any particular objective standard in the strength of attack, relative strength of overtones etc. that defines them as harsh.
I expect that an analysis of the spectrum of one single "beautiful" tone produced by a professional pianist, not a machine, at any rate of volume or loudness will prove that it has a certain ratio of rich, harmonic overtones in the lower range (the natural overtones of the vibrating string) and a minimum of high, discordant, dissonant overtones (the imposed overtones caused by excessive noise, not directly related to the natural vibrations of the string itself). A good tone will also last longer, i.e. "die out" more slowly than a bad one, something that can be experienced when one practises v-e-r-y s-l-o-w-l-y.Also, as to what was "conclusively" proven about hammer velocity: it wasn't. Nobody simply bothered to do anything for the side the musicians are on. For starters, two articles: 1) The pianist and the touch (one of 5 lectures on the accoustics of the piano by Swedish technicians, who try to explain the "thump" in certain sounds by certain pianists);2) Tone (one of 3 essays on piano tone and technique). This one attacks the assumptions of the existing research and does that quite convincingly.P.S.: I will now take pianoman53's advice to heart and won't be bothering anymore in this thread.
The immense control of tonal power in the mid-range is one of the distinguishing factors between great pianists and average ones. Nyiregyhazi is a prime example of this in that recording of the 3rd HR. This control doesn't come from striking the keys. It comes from having immense control over the hammers themselves! Intensity of tone and loudness of sound are not the same thing.