I think the word 'reading' throws me a little when used for music. When we read words we say the word internally, you can't help but say it even when you quickly glance at a word.
I think the word 'reading' throws me a little when used for music. When we read words we say the word internally, you can't help but say it even when you quickly glance at a word. When you read a word the words meaning is understood, and its this understanding of 'meaning' that I view as 'reading'Music is different and by nature is based on sound. So what does it mean to you to read music? Do you say the note names internally when reading? Do you hear the melody accurately or is it a vague representation of what is written?
Which leads us to the notion that it would be unreasonable to expect to comprehend and understand music similarly to the way you do English until you have spoken as much music as you have English - but very few of us will ever do that.
I really envy and admire those who can look at a score and hear it in their head, just as I can look at a poem and hear it in my head. I have had some ear training, and I'm a decent sight singer, but I am in awe of folks who can read a score in that way with easy fluency.
Not necessarily when you are a fast and fluent reader. You do chunking and skipping a lot just as people do when sight reading.
I think the term is really used in two different -- very different -- ways. First, of course is "sight reading" -- generally taken to mean taking a score more or less without having ever seen it before, and being able to play it at a reasonable tempo with a reasonable approximation to the form -- melody and harmony -- even if without quite all the notes and nuances.The second way is very different: I would use it to mean taking a musical score -- either for a solo instrument or for a group (very often, in my past career, a choir) and being able to hear what the music will sound like when you get the group (or instrument) rehearsed to the point of performance. It is an art, but it is at least partly learnable... with a lot of practice.
It depends what you mean by "say". When I see a film with subtitles, it's impossible for me not to process the words in my head if I glance anywhere near them. I don't "say" them internally in the sense that I would if trying to slowly memorise a sentence. However, i can't fail to process the words in full if I even glance.
Due to the level of complexity in piano scores, I doubt whether anyone really reads quite as fluently as is possible with words.
It seems you just forgot the estimately 90% of people who do not speak English at all or only little
..or your native spoken language, obviously.
Speed reading is based on the ability to not do that for every word. Concentrating on individual words will often hinder full understanding. For me it's rather easy to avoid that when I am really interested in what I am reading, otherwise it's really hard and needs practice.I feel there's at least some analogy for music reading. There are things in the score you don't need to process at all (many rests for example). I assume when you get more competent you will have more of those?Probably so.
There's actually a big limit to what doesn't need to processed unless using memory. Even rests after a note must register, to time release correctly.
Vastly less assumptions are possible in music than in reading. The difference between an implied perfect cadence and a surprise interrupted can be just one note of 4 or 5. Music needs much more verification of even strong expectation to stay accurate. The most competent logically must just process quicker and more accurately.
I don't call it speed reading unless processing the words, personally. I don't "say" them in an internal voice but my eyes run across each word. I can read very fast indeed this way. I'd call it skim reading, if the eyes are merely looking for something of interest. At that point, there's so much information on the page that many words can neither be said internally nor even processed. But speed reading doesn't mean failing to process words outright, to me.
I guess sometimes one has to register a rest after a note. But there are often several rests in one hand before the next note is to be played. No need to register them all (and which ones were used to fill the measures on one staff), only the next note to play in that hand.
Nope.Good sight readers process larger chunks of information as one item, not more smaller items faster. Those rests form part of a chunk. They aren't registered individually, but then neither are the individual notes. The "next note to play" is part of a much larger picture incorporated in a chunk and isn't processed individually.
That's a common view but highly improbable. Expectation is too limited to be reliable without verification of detail. Words are much more predictable than music. Any one of many details could vary subtly compared to expectation. A perfect cadence vs interrupted is but one note different. Expectations cannot be trusted without an eye for detail. It's the eye for detail that identifies the chunk accurately, or there would be endless false assumptions. It would be like if there were infinite combinations of letters in words. Combinations of letters are more limited and thus require less detailed verification for certainty.
There was nothing in what I said that implied any level of expectation, which I take you to mean some level of educated guess. The chunk is read accurately and in full detail.
How do you read a chunk without seeing it's components- if any small change would make it something else? It's a subjective experience assembled via rapid processing of details, surely? What else could be possible? The problem is that although experienced this way, it's impossible without grounding in both comparative intervallic reading and reading of individual notes. The significance for a good reader is nil, but for a poor reader it's necessary to assemble chunks by rapid reading of details or the concept of chunks is inaccessible.
I'm probably not a good person to ask how it's done, it's just something I do. Part of it is experience - I read a lot of music - and so my collection of separately identifiable chunks is probably pretty extensive. It's not universal, of course, and there are certainly instances where I have to construct the chunks in the way you describe. I don't dispute the need for solid grounding in intervallic reading and reading of individual notes, it would seem absolutely essential.Poor readers clearly need to do the groundwork, and it's no good to point them at chunking as a way to improve, it's something that happens, but I don't think it can be forced and I'm not sure there are any shortcuts.
I think my point is that all chunks are constructed- regardless of perception. It's just that sometimes we need to perceive the assembly and sometimes we don't. Expectation does play a big role but good readers are better at verifying or discarding assumptions rather than running blind. If a complex chord with odd intervals and many accidentals were to recur in many pieces of music, sometimes in the exact same form sometimes with a small chnge, you'd have to take tremendous care not to make errors. You couldn't simply use memory or you'd miss changes. It would take as much Care to genuinely verify the familiar weird chord (without mistaken positives) as to correctly read the one with a single different note. It's not about familiarity but the complexity involved in a true verification. Memory could start to assume based on incomplete information but it couldn't verify the chord without seeing each individual component. Chunks that we read at a glance are more basic. Logically, it all points to good readers not so much using memory to read familiar chunks as using rapid processing of detail to to assemble a picture and then compare that against memories. It's not quite the same as identifying something quick because of memory.
I think to some extent what I do, at least, is read the differences as much as the familiar. So for me, that complex chord would be a template, and any difference - one note, even - would be what I consciously saw - as strongly as the chunkised chord. It's not a matter of constructing, or reconstructing the familiar from scratch, however quickly, as recognising how what is there on the page differs from an inbuilt pattern.
I'm not so convinced.
That's what I actually meant, but you said it better. Same with text, at some level of consciousness you do probably register the unnecessary things as well, just not to the similar detail. Just like when you sight read, when I read text really efficiently and fast I am nor fully aware of what I do, it's just natural.
I suspect you could say its to do with having a certain familiarity with the syntax of the language. We know how to predict the structure of sentences and paragraphs and so long as small details are done correctly we brush over them as they were at least partially known in advance.This is true of music, once we have a familiarity with a genre or style, common chord progressions and scales, the ability to recognise themes/motifs and developments of those etc. Well written music also often has leading tones that tell us roughly what the next development will be before it happens.